-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 0
/
barela_etal_2024.bib
1493 lines (1406 loc) · 164 KB
/
barela_etal_2024.bib
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
658
659
660
661
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
675
676
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
710
711
712
713
714
715
716
717
718
719
720
721
722
723
724
725
726
727
728
729
730
731
732
733
734
735
736
737
738
739
740
741
742
743
744
745
746
747
748
749
750
751
752
753
754
755
756
757
758
759
760
761
762
763
764
765
766
767
768
769
770
771
772
773
774
775
776
777
778
779
780
781
782
783
784
785
786
787
788
789
790
791
792
793
794
795
796
797
798
799
800
801
802
803
804
805
806
807
808
809
810
811
812
813
814
815
816
817
818
819
820
821
822
823
824
825
826
827
828
829
830
831
832
833
834
835
836
837
838
839
840
841
842
843
844
845
846
847
848
849
850
851
852
853
854
855
856
857
858
859
860
861
862
863
864
865
866
867
868
869
870
871
872
873
874
875
876
877
878
879
880
881
882
883
884
885
886
887
888
889
890
891
892
893
894
895
896
897
898
899
900
901
902
903
904
905
906
907
908
909
910
911
912
913
914
915
916
917
918
919
920
921
922
923
924
925
926
927
928
929
930
931
932
933
934
935
936
937
938
939
940
941
942
943
944
945
946
947
948
949
950
951
952
953
954
955
956
957
958
959
960
961
962
963
964
965
966
967
968
969
970
971
972
973
974
975
976
977
978
979
980
981
982
983
984
985
986
987
988
989
990
991
992
993
994
995
996
997
998
999
1000
@article{Addessi.etal.2013,
title = {Delay choice versus delay maintenance: {{Different}} measures of delayed gratification in capuchin monkeys ({{{\emph{Cebus}}}}{\emph{ apella}})},
shorttitle = {Delay choice versus delay maintenance},
author = {Addessi, Elsa and Paglieri, Fabio and Beran, Michael J. and Evans, Theodore A. and Macchitella, Luigi and De Petrillo, Francesca and Focaroli, Valentina},
year = {2013},
journal = {Journal of Comparative Psychology},
volume = {127},
number = {4},
pages = {392--398},
issn = {1939-2087(Electronic);0735-7036(Print)},
doi = {10.1037/a0031869},
abstract = {Delaying gratification involves 2 components: (1) delay choice (selecting a delayed reward over an immediate one) and (2) delay maintenance (sustaining the decision to delay gratification even if the immediate reward is available during the delay). Two tasks most commonly have explored these components in primates: the intertemporal choice task and the accumulation task. It is unclear whether these tasks provide equivalent measures of delay of gratification. Here, we compared the performance on the intertemporal choice task and the accumulation task of capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) belonging to 2 study populations. We found only limited evidence of a significant correlation in performance. Consequently, in contrast to what is often assumed, our data provide only partial support for the hypothesis that these tasks provide equivalent measures of delay of gratification.},
copyright = {(c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved},
keywords = {*Delay of Gratification,*Monkeys,Choice behavior}
}
@article{Amici.etal.2019,
title = {The ability to recognize dog emotions depends on the cultural milieu in which we grow up},
author = {Amici, Federica and Waterman, James and Kellermann, Christina Maria and Karimullah, Karimullah and Br{\"a}uer, Juliane},
year = {2019},
month = nov,
journal = {Scientific Reports},
volume = {9},
number = {1},
pages = {1--9},
issn = {2045-2322},
doi = {10.1038/s41598-019-52938-4},
urldate = {2019-11-13},
abstract = {Inter-specific emotion recognition is especially adaptive when species spend a long time in close association, like dogs and humans. Here, we comprehensively studied the human ability to recognize facial expressions associated with dog emotions (hereafter, emotions). Participants were presented with pictures of dogs, humans and chimpanzees, showing angry, fearful, happy, neutral and sad emotions, and had to assess which emotion was shown, and the context in which the picture had been taken. Participants were recruited among children and adults with different levels of general experience with dogs, resulting from different personal (i.e. dog ownership) and cultural experiences (i.e. growing up or being exposed to a cultural milieu in which dogs are highly valued and integrated in human lives). Our results showed that some dog emotions such as anger and happiness are recognized from early on, independently of experience. However, the ability to recognize dog emotions is mainly acquired through experience. In adults, the probability of recognizing dog emotions was higher for participants grown up in a cultural milieu with a positive attitude toward dogs, which may result in different passive exposure, interest or inclination toward this species.},
copyright = {2019 The Author(s)},
langid = {english}
}
@article{Aria.etal.2021,
title = {The scholar's best friend: research trends in dog cognitive and behavioral studies},
shorttitle = {The scholar's best friend},
author = {Aria, Massimo and Alterisio, Alessandra and Scandurra, Anna and Pinelli, Claudia and D'Aniello, Biagio},
year = {2021},
month = may,
journal = {Animal Cognition},
volume = {24},
number = {3},
pages = {541--553},
issn = {1435-9456},
doi = {10.1007/s10071-020-01448-2},
urldate = {2021-05-18},
abstract = {In recent decades, cognitive and behavioral knowledge in dogs seems to have developed considerably, as deduced from the published peer-reviewed articles. However, to date, the worldwide trend of scientific research on dog cognition and behavior has never been explored using a bibliometric approach, while the evaluation of scientific research has increasingly become important in recent years. In this review, we compared the publication trend of the articles in the last 34~years on dogs' cognitive and behavioral science with those in the general category ``Behavioral Science''. We found that, after 2005, there has been a sharp increase in scientific publications on dogs. Therefore, the year 2005 has been used as ``starting point'' to perform an in-depth bibliometric analysis of the scientific activity in dog cognitive and behavioral studies. The period between 2006 and 2018 is taken as the study period, and a backward analysis was also carried out. The data analysis was performed using ``bibliometrix'', a new R-tool used for comprehensive science mapping analysis. We analyzed all information related to sources, countries, affiliations, co-occurrence network, thematic maps, collaboration network, and world map. The results scientifically support the common perception that dogs are attracting the interest of scholars much more now than before and more than the general trend in cognitive and behavioral studies. Both, the changes in research themes and new research themes, contributed to the increase in the scientific production on the cognitive and behavioral aspects of dogs. Our investigation may benefit the researchers interested in the field of cognitive and behavioral science in dogs, thus favoring future research work and promoting interdisciplinary collaborations.},
langid = {english},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Aria et al/aria_et_al_2021_the_scholar’s_best_friend.pdf;/home/jstevens/Zotero/storage/62WZYDKP/Aria et al. - 2020 - The scholar’s best friend research trends in dog .pdf;/home/jstevens/Zotero/storage/H9Q6SBM7/Aria et al. - 2021 - The scholar’s best friend research trends in dog .pdf}
}
@misc{Barela.etal.2023,
title = {Impulsivity as a trait in domestic dogs ({{Canis}} familiaris): {{A}} systematic review and meta-analysis},
shorttitle = {Impulsivity as a trait in domestic dogs ({{Canis}} familiaris)},
author = {Barela, Jessica and Worth, Yasmin and Stevens, Jeffrey R.},
year = {2023},
month = jan,
publisher = {{PsyArXiv}},
doi = {10.31234/osf.io/ctfns},
urldate = {2023-01-06},
abstract = {Impulsivity is a critical component of dog (Canis familiaris) behavior that owners often want to curtail. Though studies of dog impulsivity have examined their inability to wait and to inhibit inappropriate behaviors, it is not clear whether impulsivity is a behavioral trait with consistent characteristics across contexts. For this project, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis to investigate whether impulsivity exists as a behavioral trait in domestic dogs. Under a pre-registered protocol, we processed over 10,000 bibliographic database records to uncover 13 articles with multiple impulsivity tasks assessed in the same subjects. Across 31 pairs of impulsivity tasks, 28 failed to detect a correlation in performance between tasks and 3 detected a correlation. For 15 correlations of impulsivity tasks with the owner's perception of their dog's impulsivity, 10 were not correlated, while 5 were correlated. A formal meta-analysis on one pair of tasks (A-not-B task and Cylinder task) tested across seven different studies showed no overall correlation between the tasks. Our systematic review and meta-analysis found little indication of consistent relationships between impulsivity levels across tasks for dogs. Therefore, at the moment, we do not have good evidence of impulsivity as a behavioral trait that transfers across contexts, suggesting that perhaps we should focus on the context-specific nature of impulsivity in dogs.},
copyright = {Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License (CC-BY-NC-SA)},
langid = {american},
keywords = {Animal Learning and Behavior,canine,Cognitive Psychology,dog,impulsivity,inhibitory control,Judgment and Decision Making,Life Sciences,meta-analysis,Meta-science,Problem Solving,Social and Behavioral Sciences,systematic review},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Barela et al/barela_et_al_2023_impulsivity_as_a_trait_in_domestic_dogs_(canis_familiaris).pdf}
}
@article{Barrera.etal.2019,
title = {Training improves inhibitory control in water rescue dogs},
author = {Barrera, Gabriela and Alterisio, Alessandra and Scandurra, Anna and Bentosela, Mariana and D'Aniello, Biagio},
year = {2019},
month = jan,
journal = {Animal Cognition},
volume = {22},
number = {1},
pages = {127--131},
issn = {1435-9456},
doi = {10.1007/s10071-018-1224-9},
urldate = {2019-01-18},
abstract = {Inhibitory control is a collection of several processes that are aimed to refrain from any impulsive response in the subject during inappropriate situations. Evidence suggests that in dogs, the inhibitory control is affected by domestication process, but also experiences during ontogeny could be an important driver in acquiring inhibitory control. The aim of the study was to compare the performance of highly trained dogs (i.e., water rescue dogs) and pet dogs in the A-not-B task. In this procedure, the animals have to inhibit their urge of going to a previous reinforced place. The results showed that the trained dogs committed fewer errors in the task than the pet dogs suggesting a better inhibitory control. This result could indicate that inhibitory control is a flexible ability affected by ontogenetic processes such as the training experience.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {A-not-B task,Inhibitory control,Pet dogs.,Water rescue dogs}
}
@article{Bartos.etal.2022,
title = {Adjusting for publication bias in {{JASP}} and {{R}}: selection models, {{PET-PEESE}}, and robust {{Bayesian}} meta-analysis},
shorttitle = {Adjusting for publication bias in {{JASP}} and {{R}}},
author = {Barto{\v s}, Franti{\v s}ek and Maier, Maximilian and Quintana, Daniel S. and Wagenmakers, Eric-Jan},
year = {2022},
month = jul,
journal = {Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science},
volume = {5},
number = {3},
pages = {25152459221109259},
publisher = {{SAGE Publications Inc}},
issn = {2515-2459},
doi = {10.1177/25152459221109259},
urldate = {2022-09-02},
abstract = {Meta-analyses are essential for cumulative science, but their validity can be compromised by publication bias. To mitigate the impact of publication bias, one may apply publication-bias-adjustment techniques such as precision-effect test and precision-effect estimate with standard errors (PET-PEESE) and selection models. These methods, implemented in JASP and R, allow researchers without programming experience to conduct state-of-the-art publication-bias-adjusted meta-analysis. In this tutorial, we demonstrate how to conduct a publication-bias-adjusted meta-analysis in JASP and R and interpret the results. First, we explain two frequentist bias-correction methods: PET-PEESE and selection models. Second, we introduce robust Bayesian meta-analysis, a Bayesian approach that simultaneously considers both PET-PEESE and selection models. We illustrate the methodology on an example data set, provide an instructional video (https://bit.ly/pubbias) and an R-markdown script (https://osf.io/uhaew/), and discuss the interpretation of the results. Finally, we include concrete guidance on reporting the meta-analytic results in an academic article.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {model averaging,PET-PEESE,publication bias,robust Bayesian meta-analysis,selection models},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Bartoš et al/bartoš_et_al_2022_adjusting_for_publication_bias_in_jasp_and_r.pdf}
}
@article{Batt.etal.2008,
title = {Factors associated with success in guide dog training},
author = {Batt, Lara S. and Batt, Marjolyn S. and Baguley, John A. and McGreevy, Paul D.},
year = {2008},
month = jul,
journal = {Journal of Veterinary Behavior},
volume = {3},
number = {4},
pages = {143--151},
issn = {1558-7878},
doi = {10.1016/j.jveb.2008.04.003},
urldate = {2022-03-25},
abstract = {Tests of motor laterality and behavioral reactivity, as well as salivary cortisol concentrations, were examined in this pilot study to identify dogs best suited to guide dog work. Over a 14-month period, lateralization tests were conducted and cortisol concentrations were determined on 3 separate occasions, and temperament testing was performed on 2. Potential guide dogs (N = 43) involved in this study were 5 golden retrievers (4 males, 1 female) and thirty-eight Labrador retrievers (8 black males, fifteen yellow males, 5 black females, and ten yellow females). Results from these tests were then compared with the ultimate success of the dogs in the Guide Dogs NSW/ACT training program. This comparison produced evidence that motor lateralization (particularly the rate at which both paws were used during the Kong Test and the lateralization index during the Tape Test), reactions to an unfamiliar dog, the latency for dogs to drop and rest during an uninterrupted period, and the dog's color and breed were predictive of ultimate success. This study also identified 14 months of age as a more accurate time to assess dogs for these traits than either 6 months of age or at the age at which they completed their training (ranging from 14 to 20 months of age).},
langid = {english},
keywords = {cortisol,dog,guide dog,motor lateralization,temperament},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Batt et al/batt_et_al_2008_factors_associated_with_success_in_guide_dog_training.pdf}
}
@incollection{Bensky.etal.2013,
title = {The world from a dog's point of view: a review and synthesis of dog cognition research},
shorttitle = {Chapter {{Five}} - {{The World}} from a {{Dog}}'s {{Point}} of {{View}}},
booktitle = {Advances in the {{Study}} of {{Behavior}}},
author = {Bensky, Miles K. and Gosling, Samuel D. and Sinn, David L.},
editor = {Brockmann, H. Jane and Roper, Timothy J. and Naguib, Marc and Mitani, John C. and Simmons, Leigh W. and Barrett, Louise},
year = {2013},
month = jan,
volume = {45},
pages = {209--406},
publisher = {{Academic Press}},
doi = {10.1016/B978-0-12-407186-5.00005-7},
urldate = {2017-10-27},
abstract = {Driven by both applied and theoretical goals, scientific interest in canine cognition has experienced a rapid surge in popularity, especially over the last 15 years. Here we provide the most comprehensive review to date of dog cognition research, capturing all the articles (285) we could find on the subject going back to 1911. We begin by summarizing the general research trends, first documenting the rapid recent growth in dog cognition research (particularly in the domain of social cognition), and then identifying a number of trends in terms of the cognition topics and dog populations studied. Next, we summarize and synthesize the substantive conclusions emerging from research on nonsocial (discrimination learning, object permanence, object learning, categorization, object manipulation, quantitative understanding, spatial cognition, and memory) and social (responses to human cues, perspective taking, dog-human communication, and social learning) cognition. In light of the burgeoning research on individual differences in cognition and on the biological organization of cognitive domains, we highlight the potential impact of these topics on the dog cognition field. Finally, based on our syntheses, we outline some ideas for future research, including recommendations that studies focus on: (1) incorporating multiple sensory modalities (most notably olfaction); (2) using more diverse populations of subjects; (3) replicating studies where current knowledge is based on small study sets or on small samples; (4) identifying fundamental developmental patterns of cognitive development; (5) identifying individual differences in cognitive ability; and (6) identifying potential cognitive constraints (e.g. cognitive abilities that are nonindependent due to pleiotropic biological organization).},
keywords = {Canine,Cognition,Dog,Individual differences,Intelligence,Social}
}
@article{Bertoldo.etal.2022,
title = {Designing studies and evaluating research results: {{Type M}} and type {{S}} errors for pearson correlation coefficient},
shorttitle = {Designing studies and evaluating research results},
author = {Bertoldo, Giulia and Callegher, Claudio Zandonella and Alto{\`e}, Gianmarco},
year = {2022},
month = jan,
journal = {Meta-Psychology},
volume = {6},
issn = {2003-2714},
doi = {10.15626/MP.2020.2573},
urldate = {2023-01-01},
abstract = {It is widely appreciated that many studies in psychological science suffer from low statistical power. One of the consequences of analyzing underpowered studies with thresholds of statistical significance is a high risk of finding exaggerated effect size estimates, in the right or the wrong direction. These inferential risks can be directly quantified in terms of Type M (magnitude) error and Type S (sign) error, which directly communicate the consequences of design choices on effect size estimation. Given a study design, Type M error is the factor by which a statistically significant effect is on average exaggerated. Type S error is the probability to find a statistically significant result in the opposite direction to the plausible one. Ideally, these errors should be considered during a prospective design analysis in the design phase of a study to determine the appropriate sample size. However, they can also be considered when evaluating studies' results in a retrospective design analysis. In the present contribution, we aim to facilitate the considerations of these errors in the research practice in psychology. For this reason, we illustrate how to consider Type M and Type S errors in a design analysis using one of the most common effect size measures in psychology: Pearson correlation coefficient. We provide various examples and make the R functions freely available to enable researchers to perform design analysis for their research projects.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Correlation coefficient,Design analysis,Effect size,Type M error,Type S error},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Bertoldo et al/bertoldo_et_al_2022_designing_studies_and_evaluating_research_results.pdf}
}
@article{Blanchard.Hayden.2015,
title = {Monkeys are more patient in a foraging task than in a standard intertemporal choice task},
author = {Blanchard, Tommy C. and Hayden, Benjamin Y.},
year = {2015},
month = feb,
journal = {PLoS ONE},
volume = {10},
number = {2},
pages = {e0117057},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0117057},
urldate = {2015-02-17},
abstract = {Studies of animal impulsivity generally find steep subjective devaluation, or discounting, of delayed rewards \textendash{} often on the order of a 50\% reduction in value in a few seconds. Because such steep discounting is highly disfavored in evolutionary models of time preference, we hypothesize that discounting tasks provide a poor measure of animals' true time preferences. One prediction of this hypothesis is that estimates of time preferences based on these tasks will lack external validity, i.e. fail to predict time preferences in other contexts. We examined choices made by four rhesus monkeys in a computerized patch-leaving foraging task interleaved with a standard intertemporal choice task. Monkeys were significantly more patient in the foraging task than in the intertemporal choice task. Patch-leaving behavior was well fit by parameter-free optimal foraging equations but poorly fit by the hyperbolic discount parameter obtained from the intertemporal choice task. Day-to-day variation in time preferences across the two tasks was uncorrelated with each other. These data are consistent with the conjecture that seemingly impulsive behavior in animals is an artifact of their difficulty understanding the structure of intertemporal choice tasks, and support the idea that animals are more efficient rate maximizers in the multi-second range than intertemporal choice tasks would suggest.}
}
@article{Bradshaw.Goodwin.1999,
title = {Determination of behavioural traits of pure-bred dogs using factor analysis and cluster analysis; a comparison of studies in the {{USA}} and {{UK}}},
author = {Bradshaw, J. W. S and Goodwin, D},
year = {1999},
month = jan,
journal = {Research in Veterinary Science},
volume = {66},
number = {1},
pages = {73--76},
issn = {0034-5288},
doi = {10.1053/rvsc.1998.0246},
urldate = {2022-03-03},
abstract = {The questionnaire survey of Hart and Hart (1985,Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association186, 1811\textendash 1815) ranked the 56 most popular breeds of dog in theUSAon 13 behavioural traits and is compared here with results of a similar survey conducted on the 49 most popular breeds in theUK. Of the 36 breeds in common between the studies, 24 were similar for the traits aggressivity, reactivity and ease of housetraining between the two countries. However, the characteristics of nine breeds (Airedale Terrier, Old English Sheepdog, Welsh Corgi, Irish Setter, Standard Poodle, Beagle, Samoyed, Boxer, Dalmatian) differed markedly between the two countries, and a further three (Chihuahua, Scottish Terrier, Standard Dachshund) showed smaller, but probably meaningful, shifts. These differences should be recognised when giving advice to prospective owners, and when treating unwanted behaviour in these breeds.},
langid = {english},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Bradshaw et al/bradshaw_et_al_1999_determination_of_behavioural_traits_of_pure-bred_dogs_using_factor_analysis_and.pdf}
}
@article{Brady.etal.2018,
title = {A spatial discounting test to assess impulsivity in dogs},
author = {Brady, Karen and Hewison, Lynn and Wright, Hannah and Zulch, Helen and Cracknell, Nina and Mills, Daniel},
year = {2018},
month = may,
journal = {Applied Animal Behaviour Science},
volume = {202},
pages = {77--84},
issn = {0168-1591},
doi = {10.1016/j.applanim.2018.01.003},
urldate = {2018-05-04},
abstract = {In domestic dog's trait impulsivity can be measured psychometrically using the Dog Impulsivity Assessment Scale (DIAS) and experimentally using a temporal discounting paradigm which requires substantial training. A Spatial Discounting Task (SDT) was developed as an alternative experimental method to assess impulsivity, and evaluated performance in adult (2\textendash 10 years) and younger (2\textendash 9 months) dogs. The test was modified for field use with fewer controls (Simplified Spatial Discounting Task (SDTs)). Convergent validity with the SDT and DIAS Overall Questionnaire Scores (OQS) and stability over time (4\textendash 6 weeks) in the two age groups was determined. 96\% of dogs recruited reached criterion for testing. A significant positive relationship was found between Maximum Distance Travelled (MDT) in the SDT and OQS in adult dogs (r\,=\,0.46, p\,=\,0.028), with good test-retest reliability evident for both (p\,{$<$}\,0.001). In young dogs, there was good test-retest reliability for OQS (p\,=\,0.023), but no significant relationship was found between OQS and MDT, test-retest reliability for MDT in young dogs was poor. In the SDTs, 100\% of dogs recruited met criterion for testing and there was a significant relationship between MDT and OQS (r\,=\,0.61, p\,=\,0.027). The SDT appears to be a useful method for measuring impulsivity in adult dogs with wide applicability.},
keywords = {Delayed reward,Impulsivity,Spatial discounting,Temperament},
file = {/home/jstevens/Zotero/storage/FUTPJ72Q/brady_et_al_2018_a_spatial_discounting_test_to_assess_impulsivity_in_dogs.pdf}
}
@article{Bray.etal.2014,
title = {Context specificity of inhibitory control in dogs},
author = {Bray, Emily E. and MacLean, Evan L. and Hare, Brian A.},
year = {2014},
month = jan,
journal = {Animal Cognition},
volume = {17},
number = {1},
pages = {15--31},
issn = {1435-9448, 1435-9456},
doi = {10.1007/s10071-013-0633-z},
urldate = {2014-02-04},
abstract = {Across three experiments, we explored whether a dog's capacity for inhibitory control is stable or variable across decision-making contexts. In the social task, dogs were first exposed to the reputations of a stingy experimenter that never shared food and a generous experimenter who always shared food. In subsequent test trials, dogs were required to avoid approaching the stingy experimenter when this individual offered (but withheld) a higher-value reward than the generous experimenter did. In the A-not-B task, dogs were required to inhibit searching for food in a previously rewarded location after witnessing the food being moved from this location to a novel hiding place. In the cylinder task, dogs were required to resist approaching visible food directly (because it was behind a transparent barrier), in favor of a detour reaching response. Overall, dogs exhibited inhibitory control in all three tasks. However, individual scores were not correlated between tasks, suggesting that context has a large effect on dogs' behavior. This result mirrors studies of humans, which have highlighted intra-individual variation in inhibitory control as a function of the decision-making context. Lastly, we observed a correlation between a subject's age and performance on the cylinder task, corroborating previous observations of age-related decline in dogs' executive function.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Behavioural Sciences,Canine,Cognition,Domestic dogs,evolutionary biology,Human Physiology,Inhibitory control,Zoology}
}
@article{Bray.etal.2015,
title = {Increasing arousal enhances inhibitory control in calm but not excitable dogs},
author = {Bray, Emily E. and MacLean, Evan L. and Hare, Brian A.},
year = {2015},
month = nov,
journal = {Animal Cognition},
volume = {18},
number = {6},
pages = {1317--1329},
issn = {1435-9456},
doi = {10.1007/s10071-015-0901-1},
urldate = {2019-08-23},
abstract = {The emotional-reactivity hypothesis proposes that problem-solving abilities can be constrained by temperament, within and across species. One way to test this hypothesis is with the predictions of the Yerkes\textendash Dodson law. The law posits that arousal level, a component of temperament, affects problem solving in an inverted U-shaped relationship: Optimal performance is reached at intermediate levels of arousal and impeded by high and low levels. Thus, a powerful test of the emotional-reactivity hypothesis is to compare cognitive performance in dog populations that have been bred and trained based in part on their arousal levels. We therefore compared a group of pet dogs to a group of assistance dogs bred and trained for low arousal (N = 106) on a task of inhibitory control involving a detour response. Consistent with the Yerkes\textendash Dodson law, assistance dogs, which began the test with lower levels of baseline arousal, showed improvements when arousal was artificially increased. In contrast, pet dogs, which began the test with higher levels of baseline arousal, were negatively affected when their arousal was increased. Furthermore, the dogs' baseline levels of arousal, as measured in their rate of tail wagging, differed by population in the expected directions. Low-arousal assistance dogs showed the most inhibition in a detour task when humans eagerly encouraged them, while more highly aroused pet dogs performed worst on the same task with strong encouragement. Our findings support the hypothesis that selection on temperament can have important implications for cognitive performance.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Arousal,Assistance dogs,Canine,Cognition,Inhibitory control},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Bray et al/bray_et_al_2015_increasing_arousal_enhances_inhibitory_control_in_calm_but_not_excitable_dogs.pdf}
}
@article{Brucks.etal.2017a,
title = {Measures of dogs' inhibitory control abilities do not correlate across tasks},
author = {Brucks, D{\'e}sir{\'e}e and {Marshall-Pescini}, Sarah and Wallis, Lisa Jessica and Huber, Ludwig and Range, Friederike},
year = {2017},
journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
volume = {8},
pages = {849},
issn = {1664-1078},
doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00849},
urldate = {2017-10-17},
abstract = {Inhibitory control, the ability to overcome prepotent but ineffective behaviors, has been studied extensively across species, revealing the involvement of this ability in many different aspects of life. While various different paradigms have been created in order to measure inhibitory control, only a limited number of studies have investigated whether such measurements indeed evaluate the same underlying mechanism, especially in non-human animals. In humans, inhibitory control is a complex construct composed of distinct behavioral processes rather than of a single unified measure. In the current study, we aimed to investigate the validity of inhibitory control paradigms in dogs. Sixty-seven dogs were tested in a battery consisting of frequently used inhibitory control tests. Additionally, dog owners were asked to complete an impulsivity questionnaire about their dog. No correlation of dogs' performance across tasks was found. In order to understand whether there are some underlying behavioral aspects explaining dogs' performance across tests, we performed principle component analyses. Results revealed that three components (persistency, compulsivity and decision speed) explained the variation across tasks. The questionnaire and dogs' individual characteristics (i.e. age and sex) provided only limited information for the derived components. Overall, results suggest that no unique measurement for inhibitory control exists in dogs, but tests rather measure different aspects of this ability. Considering the context-specificity of inhibitory control in dogs and most probably also in other non-human animals, extreme caution is needed when making conclusions about inhibitory control abilities based on a single test.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Delay of Gratification,Dogs,Inhibitory Control,Persistency,Test battery}
}
@article{Brucks.etal.2017b,
title = {Reward type and behavioural patterns predict dogs' success in a delay of gratification paradigm},
author = {Brucks, D{\'e}sir{\'e}e and Soliani, Matteo and Range, Friederike and {Marshall-Pescini}, Sarah},
year = {2017},
month = mar,
journal = {Scientific Reports},
volume = {7},
pages = {42459},
issn = {2045-2322},
doi = {10.1038/srep42459},
urldate = {2017-03-10},
abstract = {Inhibiting an immediate behaviour in favour of an alternative but more advantageous behaviour has been linked to individual success in life, especially in humans.},
copyright = {\textcopyright{} 2017 Macmillan Publishers Limited, part of Springer Nature. All rights reserved.},
langid = {english}
}
@article{Brucks.etal.2019,
title = {Dogs and wolves do not differ in their inhibitory control abilities in a non-social test battery},
author = {Brucks, D{\'e}sir{\'e}e and {Marshall-Pescini}, Sarah and Range, Friederike},
year = {2019},
month = jan,
journal = {Animal Cognition},
volume = {22},
number = {1},
pages = {1--15},
issn = {1435-9456},
doi = {10.1007/s10071-018-1216-9},
urldate = {2019-01-18},
abstract = {Being able to inhibit certain behaviours is of clear advantage in various situations. In particular, it has been suggested that inhibitory control plays a role in problem-solving and cooperation. Interspecific differences in the capacity for inhibitory control have been attributed to social and ecological factors, while one additional factor, namely domestication, has received only little attention so far. Dogs are an interesting species to test the effects of socio-ecological factors and also the influence of domestication on inhibitory control abilities. While dogs might have been selected for enhanced inhibition skills during domestication, the predictions derived from their socio-ecological background are reversed. Wolves are cooperative hunters and breeders, while dogs predominately scavenge and raise their young alone, accordingly, it would be predicted that dogs show impaired inhibitory control abilities since they no longer rely on these coordinated actions. To test these hypotheses, we assessed inhibitory control abilities in dogs and wolves raised and kept under similar conditions. Moreover, considering the problem of context-specificity in inhibitory control measures, we employed a multiple-test-approach. In line with previous studies, we found that the single inhibition tests did not correlate with each other. Using an exploratory approach, we found three components that explained the variation of behaviours across tests: motivation, flexibility, and perseveration. Interestingly, these inhibition components did not differ between dogs and wolves, which contradicts the predictions based on their socio-ecological backgrounds but also suggests that at least in tasks with minimal human influence, domestication did not affect dogs' inhibitory control abilities, thus raising questions in regard to the selection processes that might have affected inhibitory control abilities during the course of domestication.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Dogs,Domestication,Inhibitory control,Test battery,Wolves}
}
@article{Bunford.etal.2019,
title = {Associations among behavioral inhibition and owner-rated attention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, and personality in the domestic dog ({{Canis}} familiaris)},
author = {Bunford, N{\'o}ra and Csibra, Barbara and Pet{\'a}k, Csenge and Ferdinandy, Bence and Mikl{\'o}si, {\'A}d{\'a}m and G{\'a}csi, M{\'a}rta},
year = {2019},
journal = {Journal of Comparative Psychology},
volume = {133},
number = {2},
pages = {233--243},
issn = {1939-2087(Electronic),0735-7036(Print)},
doi = {10.1037/com0000151},
abstract = {In humans, behavioral disinhibition is associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Limitations to rodent models of ADHD-like behaviors/symptoms may be augmented by complementary ones, such as the domestic dog. We examined associations between family dogs' (N = 29; of 14 breeds and 12 mongrels) performance on a self-developed touchscreen behavioral Go/No-Go paradigm and their owner-rated inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity, accounting for relevant covariates. A greater proportion of commission errors was associated with greater hyperactivity/impulsivity. Regardless of accuracy, relative to dogs with no previous training, those with basic training had shorter response latencies. Also, regardless of accuracy, greater confidence and extraversion were associated with shorter latencies, and greater openness was associated with longer latencies. Shorter latency to commission errors was associated with greater inattention. Findings support the dog as a model of the association between behavioral disinhibition and ADHD-like behaviors/symptoms and are early evidence of convergent validity between the behavioral paradigm and the rating scale measure in dogs. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved)},
keywords = {Animal Ethology,Animal Models,Animal Personality,Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity,Behavioral Disinhibition,Behavioral Inhibition,Dogs,Errors,Hyperkinesis,Impulsiveness,Interspecies Interaction},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Bunford et al/bunford_et_al_2019_associations_among_behavioral_inhibition_and_owner-rated_attention,.pdf}
}
@article{Carter.etal.2019,
title = {Correcting for bias in psychology: {{A}} comparison of meta-analytic methods},
shorttitle = {Correcting for bias in psychology},
author = {Carter, Evan C. and Sch{\"o}nbrodt, Felix D. and Gervais, Will M. and Hilgard, Joseph},
year = {2019},
month = jun,
journal = {Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science},
volume = {2},
number = {2},
pages = {115--144},
publisher = {{SAGE Publications Inc}},
issn = {2515-2459},
doi = {10.1177/2515245919847196},
urldate = {2022-06-06},
abstract = {Publication bias and questionable research practices in primary research can lead to badly overestimated effects in meta-analysis. Methodologists have proposed a variety of statistical approaches to correct for such overestimation. However, it is not clear which methods work best for data typically seen in psychology. Here, we present a comprehensive simulation study in which we examined how some of the most promising meta-analytic methods perform on data that might realistically be produced by research in psychology. We simulated several levels of questionable research practices, publication bias, and heterogeneity, and used study sample sizes empirically derived from the literature. Our results clearly indicated that no single meta-analytic method consistently outperformed all the others. Therefore, we recommend that meta-analysts in psychology focus on sensitivity analyses\textemdash that is, report on a variety of methods, consider the conditions under which these methods fail (as indicated by simulation studies such as ours), and then report how conclusions might change depending on which conditions are most plausible. Moreover, given the dependence of meta-analytic methods on untestable assumptions, we strongly recommend that researchers in psychology continue their efforts to improve the primary literature and conduct large-scale, preregistered replications. We provide detailed results and simulation code at https://osf.io/rf3ys and interactive figures at http://www.shinyapps.org/apps/metaExplorer/.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {bias correction,meta-analysis,open data,open materials,p-hacking,publication bias,questionable research practices},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Carter et al/carter_et_al_2019_correcting_for_bias_in_psychology2.pdf;/home/jstevens/Zotero/storage/WPFKUBYA/Carter et al. - 2019 - Correcting for Bias in Psychology A Comparison of.pdf}
}
@article{Cavalli.etal.2018,
title = {Are animal-assisted activity dogs different from pet dogs? {{A}} comparison of their sociocognitive abilities},
shorttitle = {Are animal-assisted activity dogs different from pet dogs?},
author = {Cavalli, Camila Mar{\'i}a and Carballo, Fabricio and Dzik, Marina Victoria and Underwood, Susana and Bentosela, Mariana},
year = {2018},
month = jan,
journal = {Journal of Veterinary Behavior},
volume = {23},
pages = {76--81},
issn = {1558-7878},
doi = {10.1016/j.jveb.2017.12.001},
urldate = {2020-07-07},
abstract = {Animal-assisted activities (AAAs) refer to a variety of interactions between animals and humans, intended to improve people's well-being providing recreational or educational opportunities. Domestic dogs are one of the most commonly used animals for these kinds of interventions, given their trainability and the positive effects of dog-human interactions. Nevertheless, the selection of participating animals is mainly unsystematic, and training is not required for dogs to take part in AAAs. Previous studies suggest that high sociability as well as reduced fear and aggression are desirable traits in AAA dogs. Yet, to our knowledge, there are no previous studies assessing the specific characteristics of dogs participating in AAAs. The aim of this study was to compare the performance of AAA and pet dogs (PDs) that live in the same household but do not participate in AAAs. We assessed 17 dogs (9 participating in AAAs in hospital settings and 8 pets living in the same household\textendash control group) with a test battery comprising 3 behavioral tasks (sociability test, gazing test, and A-not-B task), and owner-rated questionnaires (Dog Impulsivity Assessment Scale [DIAS] and 4 subscales: trainability, fear to strangers, nonsocial fear, and attachment/attention seeking of the Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire [C-BARQ]). Results of the gazing test indicate that, when dogs were not reinforced for looking at the human face, AAA dogs gazed longer at an unknown experimenter than PDs. Therefore, they showed an increased tendency to gaze at humans and persist on this communicative attempt when this response was not successful. Additionally, according to the DIAS score, AAA dogs would be less impulsive than the control group. No significant differences were found on the A-not-B task, the sociability test, or the C-BARQ. In conclusion, since these AAA dogs had not undergone specific training, the effects observed in the present work may be attributed, at least partially, to the learning experiences they had during AAA work. Overall, it would be important to take into account these characteristics for both the selection and training of these animals.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {animal-assisted activities,domestic dogs,gazing test,inhibitory control}
}
@article{Cook.etal.2016a,
title = {Neurobehavioral evidence for individual differences in canine cognitive control: an awake {{fMRI}} study},
shorttitle = {Neurobehavioral evidence for individual differences in canine cognitive control},
author = {Cook, Peter F. and Spivak, Mark and Berns, Gregory},
year = {2016},
month = sep,
journal = {Animal Cognition},
volume = {19},
number = {5},
pages = {867--878},
issn = {1435-9456},
doi = {10.1007/s10071-016-0983-4},
urldate = {2019-01-02},
abstract = {Based on behavioral evidence, the domestic dog has emerged as a promising comparative model of human self-control. However, while research on human inhibition has probed heterogeneity and neuropathology through an integration of neural and behavioral evidence, there are no parallel data exploring the brain mechanisms involved in canine inhibition. Here, using a combination of cognitive testing and awake neuroimaging in domestic dogs, we provide evidence precisely localizing frontal brain regions underpinning response inhibition in this species and demonstrate the dynamic relationship between these regions and behavioral measures of control. Thirteen dogs took part in an in-scanner go/no-go task and an out-of-scanner A-not-B test. A frontal brain region was identified showing elevated neural activity for all subjects during successful inhibition in the scanner, and dogs showing greater mean brain activation in this region produced fewer false alarms. Better performance in the go/no-go task was also correlated with fewer errors in the out-of-scanner A-not-B test, suggesting that dogs show consistent neurobehavioral individual differences in cognitive control, as is seen in humans. These findings help establish parity between human and canine mechanisms of self-control and pave the way for future comparative studies examining their function and dysfunction.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Comparative cognition,Dog cognition,fMRI,Individual differences,Motor inhibition,Neuroimaging,Prefrontal cortex,Self-control}
}
@book{Cooper.2017,
title = {Research {{Synthesis}} and {{Meta-Analysis}}: {{A Step-by-Step Approach}}},
shorttitle = {Research {{Synthesis}} and {{Meta-Analysis}}},
author = {Cooper, Harris},
year = {2017},
publisher = {{SAGE Publications, Inc}},
address = {{2455 Teller Road,~Thousand Oaks~California~91320}},
doi = {10.4135/9781071878644},
urldate = {2022-05-02},
isbn = {978-1-4833-3115-7 978-1-07-187864-4}
}
@article{Dick.etal.2010,
title = {Understanding the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to alcohol use disorders},
shorttitle = {{{REVIEW}}},
author = {Dick, Danielle M. and Smith, Gregory and Olausson, Peter and Mitchell, Suzanne H. and Leeman, Robert F. and O'Malley, Stephanie S. and Sher, Kenneth},
year = {2010},
journal = {Addiction Biology},
volume = {15},
number = {2},
pages = {217--226},
issn = {1369-1600},
doi = {10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00190.x},
urldate = {2022-12-19},
abstract = {There are well-established links between impulsivity and alcohol use in humans and other model organisms; however, the etiological nature of these associations remains unclear. This is likely due, in part, to the heterogeneous nature of the construct of impulsivity. Many different measures of impulsivity have been employed in human studies, using both questionnaire and laboratory-based tasks. Animal studies also use multiple tasks to assess the construct of impulsivity. In both human and animal studies, different measures of impulsivity often show little correlation and are differentially related to outcome, suggesting that the impulsivity construct may actually consist of a number of more homogeneous (and potentially more meaningful) subfacets. Here, we provide an overview of the different measures of impulsivity used across human and animal studies, evidence that the construct of impulsivity may be better studied in the context of more meaningful subfacets, and recommendations for how research in this direction may provide for better consilience between human and animal studies of the connection between impulsivity and alcohol use.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Behavioral disinhibition,behavioral undercontrol,delay aversion,impulsivity,response inhibition,sensation-seeking},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Dick et al/dick_et_al_2010_review.pdf}
}
@article{Diesel.etal.2010,
title = {Survey of veterinary practice policies and opinions on neutering dogs},
author = {Diesel, G. and Brodbelt, D. and Laurence, C.},
year = {2010},
journal = {Veterinary Record},
volume = {166},
number = {15},
pages = {455--458},
issn = {2042-7670},
doi = {10.1136/vr.b4798},
urldate = {2022-03-03},
abstract = {A cross-sectional survey was conducted in 2008 to obtain information on the advice veterinary practices currently give to their clients and the opinions of veterinary surgeons regarding neutering of dogs. An additional survey of dog owners was conducted to obtain information on the neuter status of the general dog population in Great Britain. A significantly higher proportion of practices had a policy for the age of neutering bitches than for dogs. The average recommended age for neutering bitches was 6.5 months (95 per cent confidence interval [CI] 3.1 to 9.7 months) and for dogs it was 7.5 months (95 per cent CI 1.4 to 13.6 months). There was very little agreement between veterinary practices as to whether bitches should be allowed to have their first season before neutering, with 164 of 973 respondents (16.9 per cent) always recommending this, 198 (20.3 per cent) recommending it most of the time, 203 (20.9 per cent) sometimes, 203 (20.9 per cent) rarely and 201 (20.6 per cent) never recommending it. The owner study showed that 54 per cent of dogs (233 of 431) were neutered, with the North region having the lowest proportion of neutered dogs (11 of 25; 44.0 per cent).},
langid = {english},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Diesel et al/diesel_et_al_2010_survey_of_veterinary_practice_policies_and_opinions_on_neutering_dogs.pdf}
}
@article{Evenden.1999a,
title = {Varieties of impulsivity},
author = {Evenden, J L},
year = {1999},
journal = {Psychopharmacology},
volume = {146},
number = {4},
pages = {348--361},
doi = {10.1007/PL00005481},
abstract = {The concept of impulsivity covers a wide range of "actions that are poorly conceived, prematurely expressed, unduly risky, or inappropriate to the situation and that often result in undesirable outcomes". As such it plays an important role in normal behaviour, as well as, in a pathological form, in many kinds of mental illness such as mania, personality disorders, substance abuse disorders and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Although evidence from psychological studies of human personality suggests that impulsivity may be made up of several independent factors, this has: not made a major impact on biological studies of impulsivity. This may be because there is little unanimity as to which these factors are. The present review summarises evidence for varieties of impulsivity from several different areas of research: human psychology, psychiatry and animal behaviour. Recently, a series of psychopharmacological studies has been carried out by the present author and colleagues using methods proposed to measure selectively different aspects of impulsivity. The results of these studies suggest that several neurochemical mechanisms can influence impulsivity, and that impulsive behaviour has no unique neurobiological basis. Consideration of impulsivity as the result of several different, independent factors which inter act to modulate behaviour may provide better insight into the pathology than current hypotheses based on serotonergic underactivity.},
keywords = {ascending 5-hydroxytryptaminergic pathways; consecutive number schedule; reaction-time performance; interval bisection task; self-control paradigm; timing behavior; d-amphetamine; rats; lesions; attention,{impulsivity, personality, serotonin, behaviour}}
}
@article{Fadel.etal.2016,
title = {Differences in trait impulsivity indicate diversification of dog breeds into working and show lines},
author = {Fadel, Fernanda Ruiz and Driscoll, Patricia and Pilot, Malgorzata and Wright, Hannah and Zulch, Helen and Mills, Daniel},
year = {2016},
month = mar,
journal = {Scientific Reports},
volume = {6},
number = {1},
pages = {22162},
publisher = {{Nature Publishing Group}},
issn = {2045-2322},
doi = {10.1038/srep22162},
urldate = {2020-07-30},
abstract = {Impulsiveness describes the inability to inhibit behaviour in the presence of salient cues. Trait-level impulsivity exists on a continuum and individual differences can be adaptive in different contexts. While breed related differences in behavioural tendency in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) are well established, the phenomenon within lines of a breed which have been selected more recently is not well studied, although it may challenge the popular notion of breed-typical behaviour. We describe differences in impulsivity between and within two dog breeds with working and show lines selected for different levels of impulsivity: Border Collies (herding work) and Labrador Retrievers (gun work). Recent show line selection might have lessened differences in impulsivity between breeds. We tested this hypothesis on a dataset of 1161 individuals assessed using a validated psychometric tool (Dog Impulsivity Assessment Scale - DIAS). Collies were more impulsive on average, consistent with the original purpose of breed selection. Regarding line, working Collies differed from working Labradors, but show lines from the two breeds were not significantly different. Altered or relaxed artificial selection for behavioural traits when appearance rather than behaviour become the primary focus for breeders may reduce average differences in impulsivity between breeds in show lines.},
copyright = {2016 The Author(s)},
langid = {english}
}
@article{Fagnani.etal.2016,
title = {Tolerance to delayed reward tasks in social and non-social contexts},
author = {Fagnani, J. and Barrera, G. and Carballo, F. and Bentosela, M.},
year = {2016},
journal = {Behavioural Processes},
volume = {130},
pages = {19--30},
issn = {0376-6357},
doi = {10.1016/j.beproc.2016.06.011},
urldate = {2020-07-30},
abstract = {Domestic dogs have demonstrated striking social skills towards humans, however, there are few studies investigating impulsivity with delay-choice tasks in communicative contexts. In Study 1 we introduced a novel social delay-choice task in which subjects had to choose between one human cueing an immediate, low quality reward and another human signaling a delayed, high quality reward. In Study 2 we evaluated the tolerance to increasing delays using social and non-social cues. We also explored if more self-controlled dogs show any distinct behaviours during delays. Finally, we correlated all results with the Dog Impulsivity Assessment Scale (Wright et al., 2011). In Study 1 dogs reached an average maximum delay of 11.55s. In Study 2 that average was 52.14s with social cues and 40.2s with non-social, but differences were not significant. Tolerance to delays showed high interindividual variation. Dogs remained mostly standing and near the delayed experimenter in the social tasks although we could not to find any distinct coping strategies. No significant correlations were found between the delay reached and behaviours, neither with the scale. These results show the relevance of the parameters and methods used to investigate tolerance to delay of reinforcements. More investigations are required, especially an assessment of the same subjects performing the same tasks using different contexts.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Delay of reinforcement,Domestic dog,Impulsivity,Interspecific communication},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Fagnani et al/fagnani_et_al_2016_tolerance_to_delayed_reward_tasks_in_social_and_non-social_contexts.pdf}
}
@article{Fagnani.etal.2016a,
title = {Is previous experience important for inhibitory control? {{A}} comparison between shelter and pet dogs in {{A-not-B}} and cylinder tasks},
shorttitle = {Is previous experience important for inhibitory control?},
author = {Fagnani, J. and Barrera, G. and Carballo, F. and Bentosela, M.},
year = {2016},
journal = {Animal Cognition},
volume = {19},
number = {6},
pages = {1165--1172},
issn = {1435-9456},
doi = {10.1007/s10071-016-1024-z},
urldate = {2019-08-23},
abstract = {This study compares the performance of two groups of dogs with different levels of social interaction with humans, shelter and pet dogs, in two inhibitory control tasks. (1) In the A-not-B task, dogs were required to resist searching for food in a previously rewarded location, and (2) in the cylinder task, dogs were required to resist approaching visible food directly in favor of a detour reaching response. Our first aim was to evaluate the importance of learning and ontogeny in performing inhibitory tasks. Also, we assessed whether there is a correlation between the two tasks by comparing performance in the same subjects. Results showed significant differences between shelter and pet dogs in the A-not-B task, with poorer performance in shelter dogs. However, no differences were found in the cylinder task. The poorer performance of shelter dogs might be related to their infrequent interaction with humans, which reduces the chances to learn to inhibit certain behaviors. This result would highlight the importance of ontogeny in developing that ability. On the other hand, no correlations were found between the two tasks, which contributes information to the debate about the context specificity of inhibitory control in dogs.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {A-not-B task,Cylinder task,Inhibitory control,Learning,Shelter dogs}
}
@article{Fratkin.etal.2013,
title = {Personality consistency in dogs: {{A}} meta-analysis},
shorttitle = {Personality consistency in dogs},
author = {Fratkin, Jamie L. and Sinn, David L. and Patall, Erika A. and Gosling, Samuel D.},
year = {2013},
month = jan,
journal = {PLOS ONE},
volume = {8},
number = {1},
pages = {e54907},
publisher = {{Public Library of Science}},
issn = {1932-6203},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0054907},
urldate = {2022-12-02},
abstract = {Personality, or consistent individual differences in behavior, is well established in studies of dogs. Such consistency implies predictability of behavior, but some recent research suggests that predictability cannot be assumed. In addition, anecdotally, many dog experts believe that `puppy tests' measuring behavior during the first year of a dog's life are not accurate indicators of subsequent adult behavior. Personality consistency in dogs is an important aspect of human-dog relationships (e.g., when selecting dogs suitable for substance-detection work or placement in a family). Here we perform the first comprehensive meta-analysis of studies reporting estimates of temporal consistency of dog personality. A thorough literature search identified 31 studies suitable for inclusion in our meta-analysis. Overall, we found evidence to suggest substantial consistency (r = 0.43). Furthermore, personality consistency was higher in older dogs, when behavioral assessment intervals were shorter, and when the measurement tool was exactly the same in both assessments. In puppies, aggression and submissiveness were the most consistent dimensions, while responsiveness to training, fearfulness, and sociability were the least consistent dimensions. In adult dogs, there were no dimension-based differences in consistency. There was no difference in personality consistency in dogs tested first as puppies and later as adults (e.g., `puppy tests') versus dogs tested first as puppies and later again as puppies. Finally, there were no differences in consistency between working versus non-working dogs, between behavioral codings versus behavioral ratings, and between aggregate versus single measures. Implications for theory, practice, and future research are discussed.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Aggression,Animal behavior,Dogs,Metaanalysis,Personality,Personality tests,Personality traits,Pets and companion animals},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Fratkin et al/fratkin_et_al_2013_personality_consistency_in_dogs.pdf}
}
@article{Gelman.Carlin.2014,
title = {Beyond power calculations: {{Assessing}} type {{S}} (sign) and type {{M}} (magnitude) errors},
shorttitle = {Beyond power calculations},
author = {Gelman, Andrew and Carlin, John},
year = {2014},
month = nov,
journal = {Perspectives on Psychological Science},
volume = {9},
number = {6},
pages = {641--651},
publisher = {{SAGE Publications Inc}},
issn = {1745-6916},
doi = {10.1177/1745691614551642},
urldate = {2022-03-07},
abstract = {Statistical power analysis provides the conventional approach to assess error rates when designing a research study. However, power analysis is flawed in that a narrow emphasis on statistical significance is placed as the primary focus of study design. In noisy, small-sample settings, statistically significant results can often be misleading. To help researchers address this problem in the context of their own studies, we recommend design calculations in which (a) the probability of an estimate being in the wrong direction (Type S [sign] error) and (b) the factor by which the magnitude of an effect might be overestimated (Type M [magnitude] error or exaggeration ratio) are estimated. We illustrate with examples from recent published research and discuss the largest challenge in a design calculation: coming up with reasonable estimates of plausible effect sizes based on external information.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {design calculation,exaggeration ratio,power analysis,replication crisis,statistical significance,Type M error,Type S error},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Gelman et al/gelman_et_al_2014_beyond_power_calculations.pdf}
}
@article{Gnanadesikan.etal.2020,
title = {Estimating the heritability of cognitive traits across dog breeds reveals highly heritable inhibitory control and communication factors},
author = {Gnanadesikan, Gitanjali E. and Hare, Brian and {Snyder-Mackler}, Noah and MacLean, Evan L.},
year = {2020},
month = sep,
journal = {Animal Cognition},
volume = {23},
number = {5},
pages = {953--964},
issn = {1435-9456},
doi = {10.1007/s10071-020-01400-4},
urldate = {2020-08-16},
abstract = {Trait heritability is necessary for evolution by both natural and artificial selection, yet we know little about the heritability of cognitive traits. Domestic dogs are a valuable study system for questions regarding the evolution of phenotypic diversity due to their extraordinary intraspecific variation. While previous studies have investigated morphological and behavioral variation across dog breeds, few studies have systematically assessed breed differences in cognition. We integrated data from Dognition.com\textemdash a citizen science project on dog cognition\textemdash with breed-averaged genetic data from published sources to estimate the among-breed heritability of cognitive traits using mixed models. The resulting dataset included 11 cognitive measures for 1508 adult dogs across 36 breeds. A factor analysis yielded four factors interpreted as reflecting inhibitory control, communication, memory, and physical reasoning. Narrow-sense among-breed heritability estimates\textemdash reflecting the proportion of cognitive variance attributable to additive genetic variation\textemdash revealed that scores on the inhibitory control and communication factors were highly heritable (inhibitory control: h2\,=\,0.70; communication: h2\,=\,0.39), while memory and physical reasoning were less heritable (memory: h2\,=\,0.17; physical reasoning: h2\,=\,0.21). Although the heritability of inhibitory control is partially explained by body weight, controlling for breed-average weight still yields a high heritability estimate (h2\,=\,0.50), while other factors are minimally affected. Our results indicate that cognitive phenotypes in dogs covary with breed relatedness and suggest that cognitive traits have strong potential to undergo selection. The highest heritabilities were observed for inhibitory control and communication, both of which are hypothesized to have been altered by domestication.},
langid = {english}
}
@article{Guyatt.etal.2008,
title = {{{GRADE}}: an emerging consensus on rating quality of evidence and strength of recommendations},
shorttitle = {{{GRADE}}},
author = {Guyatt, Gordon H. and Oxman, Andrew D. and Vist, Gunn E. and Kunz, Regina and {Falck-Ytter}, Yngve and {Alonso-Coello}, Pablo and Sch{\"u}nemann, Holger J.},
year = {2008},
month = apr,
journal = {BMJ},
volume = {336},
number = {7650},
pages = {924--926},
publisher = {{British Medical Journal Publishing Group}},
issn = {0959-8138, 1756-1833},
doi = {10.1136/bmj.39489.470347.AD},
urldate = {2022-06-06},
abstract = {{$<$}p{$>$}Guidelines are inconsistent in how they rate the quality of evidence and the strength of recommendations. This article explores the advantages of the GRADE system, which is increasingly being adopted by organisations worldwide {$<$}/p{$>$}},
chapter = {Analysis},
copyright = {\textcopyright{} BMJ Publishing Group Ltd 2008},
langid = {english},
pmid = {18436948},
file = {/home/jstevens/Zotero/storage/JAW4M2ME/Guyatt et al. - 2008 - GRADE an emerging consensus on rating quality of .pdf}
}
@article{Haddaway.etal.2022,
title = {{{PRISMA2020}}: {{An R}} package and {{Shiny}} app for producing {{PRISMA}} 2020-compliant flow diagrams, with interactivity for optimised digital transparency and {{Open Synthesis}}},
shorttitle = {{{PRISMA2020}}},
author = {Haddaway, Neal R. and Page, Matthew J. and Pritchard, Chris C. and McGuinness, Luke A.},
year = {2022},
journal = {Campbell Systematic Reviews},
volume = {18},
number = {2},
pages = {e1230},
issn = {1891-1803},
doi = {10.1002/cl2.1230},
urldate = {2022-12-15},
abstract = {Background Reporting standards, such as PRISMA aim to ensure that the methods and results of systematic reviews are described in sufficient detail to allow full transparency. Flow diagrams in evidence syntheses allow the reader to rapidly understand the core procedures used in a review and examine the attrition of irrelevant records throughout the review process. Recent research suggests that use of flow diagrams in systematic reviews is poor and of low quality and called for standardised templates to facilitate better reporting in flow diagrams. The increasing options for interactivity provided by the Internet gives us an opportunity to support easy-to-use evidence synthesis tools, and here we report on the development of a tool for the production of PRISMA 2020-compliant systematic review flow diagrams. Methods and Findings We developed a free-to-use, Open Source R package and web-based Shiny app to allow users to design PRISMA flow diagrams for their own systematic reviews. Our tool allows users to produce standardised visualisations that transparently document the methods and results of a systematic review process in a variety of formats. In addition, we provide the opportunity to produce interactive, web-based flow diagrams (exported as HTML files), that allow readers to click on boxes of the diagram and navigate to further details on methods, results or data files. We provide an interactive example here; https://prisma-flowdiagram.github.io/. Conclusions We have developed a user-friendly tool for producing PRISMA 2020-compliant flow diagrams for users with coding experience and, importantly, for users without prior experience in coding by making use of Shiny (https://estech.shinyapps.io/prisma\_flowdiagram/). This free-to-use tool will make it easier to produce clear and PRISMA 2020-compliant systematic review flow diagrams. Significantly, users can also produce interactive flow diagrams for the first time, allowing readers of their reviews to smoothly and swiftly explore and navigate to further details of the methods and results of a review. We believe this tool will increase use of PRISMA flow diagrams, improve the compliance and quality of flow diagrams, and facilitate strong science communication of the methods and results of systematic reviews by making use of interactivity. We encourage the systematic review community to make use of the tool, and provide feedback to streamline and improve their usability and efficiency.},
copyright = {\textcopyright{} 2022 The Authors. Campbell Systematic Reviews published by John Wiley \& Sons Ltd on behalf of The Campbell Collaboration.},
langid = {english},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Haddaway et al/haddaway_et_al_2022_prisma2020.pdf}
}
@article{Hare.etal.1998,
title = {Communication of food location between human and dog ({{Canis}} familiaris)},
author = {Hare, Brian and Call, Josep and Tomasello, Michael},
year = {1998},
month = jan,
journal = {Evolution of Communication},
volume = {2},
number = {1},
pages = {137--159},
publisher = {{John Benjamins}},
issn = {1387-5337, 1569-9757},
doi = {10.1075/eoc.2.1.06har},
urldate = {2022-05-09},
abstract = {Two domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) participated in a series of studies in which they communicated with a human about the location of hidden food. In the first study both dogs were able to follow human pointing reliably to one of several locations where food was hidden, both in front of them and behind them. They also showed some skills at following human gaze direction in this same task, when both head and eyes indicated the same location. They did not follow eye direction when it conflicted with head direction. A second study clearly ruled out a low-level visual tracking explanation for at least one of the subjects. In a third study one of the two dogs was able to lead a naive human to one of three locations containing food consistently, mainly by barking and orienting its body to the food. The subject did not behave differently, however, when the human turned his back or covered his eyes; he continued to orient to the food and bark under all conditions. In a fourth study in which more clearly visual signals were involved, both subjects strongly preferred to drop a retrieved object at the front of, rather than at the back of, the human \textemdash{} even when the human turned his back so that subjects had to bring the object around his body upon return. The knowledge of human pointing and gaze direction displayed by these two domestic dogs is in many ways comparable to that displayed in experimental studies by nonhuman primates.},
langid = {english},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Hare et al/hare_et_al_1998_communication_of_food_location_between_human_and_dog_(canis_familiaris).pdf}
}
@book{Harrer.etal.2021,
title = {Doing {{Meta-Analysis}} in {{R}}: {{A Hands-On Guide}}},
author = {Harrer, Mathias and Cuijpers, Pim and Furukawa, Toshi A. and Ebert, David D.},
year = {2021},
publisher = {{Chapman \& Hall/CRC Press}},
address = {{Boca Raton, FL}},
urldate = {2021-12-20},
abstract = {This is a guide on how to conduct Meta-Analyses in R.},
isbn = {978-0-367-61007-4},
langid = {english}
}
@article{Hejjas.etal.2007,
title = {Association of polymorphisms in the dopamine {{D4}} receptor gene and the activity-impulsivity endophenotype in dogs},
author = {Hejjas, K. and Vas, J. and Topal, J. and Szantai, E. and Ronai, Z. and Szekely, A. and Kubinyi, E. and Horvath, Z. and {Sasvari-Szekely}, M. and Miklosi, A.},
year = {2007},
journal = {Animal Genetics},
volume = {38},
number = {6},
pages = {629--633},
issn = {1365-2052},
doi = {10.1111/j.1365-2052.2007.01657.x},
urldate = {2022-03-01},
abstract = {A variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) polymorphism in exon 3 of the human dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) has been associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Rodents possess no analogous repeat sequence, whereas a similar tandem repeat polymorphism of the DRD4 gene was identified in dogs, horses and chimpanzees. Here, we present a genetic association study of the DRD4 VNTR and the activity-impulsivity dimension of the recently validated dog-ADHD Rating Scale. To avoid false positives arising from population stratification, a single breed of dogs (German shepherd) was studied. Two DRD4 alleles (referred to as 2 and 3a) were detected in this breed, and genotype frequencies were in Hardy\textendash Weinberg equilibrium. For modelling distinct environmental conditions, `pet' and `police' German shepherds were characterized. Police German shepherds possessing at least one 3a allele showed significantly higher scores in the activity-impulsivity dimension of the dog-ADHD Rating Scale than dogs without this allele (P = 0.0180). This difference was not significant in pet German shepherds. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first report of an association between a candidate gene and a behaviour trait in dogs, and it reinforces the functional role of DRD4 exon 3 polymorphism.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {activity-impulsivity,dog,dopamine D4 receptor gene,genetic association,polymorphism},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Hejjas et al/hejjas_et_al_2007_association_of_polymorphisms_in_the_dopamine_d4_receptor_gene_and_the.pdf}
}
@article{Higgins.etal.2011,
title = {The {{Cochrane Collaboration}}'s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials},
author = {Higgins, Julian P. T. and Altman, Douglas G. and G{\o}tzsche, Peter C. and J{\"u}ni, Peter and Moher, David and Oxman, Andrew D. and Savovi{\'c}, Jelena and Schulz, Kenneth F. and Weeks, Laura and Sterne, Jonathan A. C.},
year = {2011},
month = oct,
journal = {BMJ},
volume = {343},
pages = {d5928},
publisher = {{British Medical Journal Publishing Group}},
issn = {0959-8138, 1468-5833},
doi = {10.1136/bmj.d5928},
urldate = {2022-06-06},
abstract = {{$<$}p{$>$}Flaws in the design, conduct, analysis, and reporting of randomised trials can cause the effect of an intervention to be underestimated or overestimated. The Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias aims to make the process clearer and more accurate{$<$}/p{$>$}},
chapter = {Research Methods \& Reporting},
copyright = {\textcopyright{} Higgins et al 2011. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-commercial License, which permits use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non commercial and is otherwise in compliance with the license. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0/ and http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0/legalcode.},
langid = {english},
pmid = {22008217},
file = {/home/jstevens/Zotero/storage/XNJ6KHF8/Higgins et al. - 2011 - The Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing ri.pdf}
}
@article{Hsu.Serpell.2003,
title = {Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs},
author = {Hsu, Yuying and Serpell, James A.},
year = {2003},
month = nov,
journal = {Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association},
volume = {223},
number = {9},
pages = {1293--1300},
issn = {0003-1488},
doi = {10.2460/javma.2003.223.1293},
urldate = {2020-01-09},
abstract = {Objective\textemdash To develop and validate a questionnaire to assess behavior and temperament traits of pet dogs. Design\textemdash Cross-sectional survey of dog owners. Animals\textemdash 1,851 dogs belonging to clients of a veterinary teaching hospital or members of national breed clubs and 203 dogs examined by canine behavior practitioners because of behavior problems. Procedure\textemdash Owners were asked to complete a questionnaire consisting of 152 items eliciting information on how dogs responded to specific events and situations in their usual environment. Data from completed questionnaires were subjected to factor analysis, and the resulting factors were tested for reliability and validity. Results\textemdash Factor analysis yielded 11 factors from 68 of the original questionnaire items that together accounted for 57\% of the common variance in questionnaire item scores. Reliability was acceptable for all but 1 of these factors. Behavior problems in 200 of the 203 dogs with behavior problems could be assigned to 7 diagnostic categories that matched 7 of the factors identified during factor analysis of questionnaire responses. Dogs assigned to particular diagnostic categories had significantly higher scores for corresponding questionnaire factors than did those assigned to unrelated diagnostic categories, indicating that the factors were valid .Validity of the remaining 4 factors could not be examined because of a lack of information on dogs with behavior problems related to these factors. Conclusions and Clinical Relevance\textemdash Findings suggest that the resulting 68-item questionnaire is a reliable and valid method of assessing behavior and temperament traits in dogs. The questionnaire may be useful in screening dogs for behavior problems and in evaluating the clinical effects of various treatments for behavior problems. (J Am Vet Med Assoc 2003;223: 1293\textendash 1300)},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Hsu et al/hsu_et_al_2003_development_and_validation_of_a_questionnaire_for_measuring_behavior_and.pdf}
}
@article{Junttila.etal.2021,
title = {Effect of sex and reproductive status on inhibitory control and social cognition in the domestic dog ({{{\emph{Canis}}}}{\emph{ familiaris}})},
author = {Junttila, Saara and Huohvanainen, Salla and Tiira, Katriina},
year = {2021},
month = aug,
journal = {Animals},
volume = {11},
number = {8},
pages = {2448},
publisher = {{Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute}},
issn = {2076-2615},
doi = {10.3390/ani11082448},
urldate = {2022-05-03},
abstract = {Sex differences in a variety of cognitive traits have long been reported in various species, including dogs. However, only a few canine studies have taken the possible effect of reproductive hormones into account. The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of sex and reproductive status of pet dogs (N = 1032) on two cognitive traits: inhibitory control and social cognition. Inhibitory control was assessed using the cylinder test, and the dogs' tendency to initiate social contact with a human during a problem-solving situation was assessed using the unsolvable task. Female dogs had a significantly higher success rate in the cylinder test compared to males, and they spent significantly more time in human-directed behavior during the unsolvable task. In contrast, males spent significantly more time in independent behavior during the unsolvable task. Reproductive status had no significant effect on the results of the cylinder test or the unsolvable task. Our results showed that female dogs asked for more help/used a more cooperative strategy during a problem-solving situation and had greater inhibitory control compared to males. According to our results, it seems likely that these sex differences were not influenced to a large extent by reproductive hormones.},
copyright = {http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/},
langid = {english},
keywords = {animal behavior,canine,cognition,cylinder test,dog,impulsivity,inhibitory control,neutering,psychology,sex differences,social cognition,unsolvable task}
}
@article{Junttila.etal.2022,
title = {Breed differences in social cognition, inhibitory control, and spatial problem-solving ability in the domestic dog ({{{\emph{Canis}}}}{\emph{ familiaris}})},
author = {Junttila, Saara and Valros, Anna and M{\"a}ki, Katariina and V{\"a}{\"a}t{\"a}j{\"a}, Heli and Reunanen, Elisa and Tiira, Katriina},
year = {2022},
month = dec,
journal = {Scientific Reports},
volume = {12},
number = {1},
pages = {22529},
publisher = {{Nature Publishing Group}},
issn = {2045-2322},
doi = {10.1038/s41598-022-26991-5},
urldate = {2022-12-29},
abstract = {The extraordinary genetic and behavioural diversity of dog breeds provides a unique opportunity for investigating the heritability of cognitive traits, such as problem-solving ability, social cognition, inhibitory control, and memory. Previous studies have mainly investigated cognitive differences between breed groups, and information on individual dog breeds is scarce. As a result, findings are often contradictory and inconsistent. The aim of this study was to provide more clarity on between-breed differences of cognitive traits in dogs. We examined the performance of 13 dog breeds (N\,=\,1002 dogs) in a standardized test battery. Significant breed differences were found for understanding of human communicative gestures, following a human's misleading gesture, spatial problem-solving ability in a V-detour task, inhibitory control in a cylinder test, and persistence and human-directed behaviour during an unsolvable task. Breeds also differed significantly in their behaviour towards an unfamiliar person, activity level, and exploration of a novel environment. No significant differences were identified in tasks measuring memory or logical reasoning. Breed differences thus emerged mainly in tasks measuring social cognition, problem-solving, and inhibitory control. Our results suggest that these traits may have come under diversifying artificial selection in different breeds. These results provide a deeper understanding on breed-specific traits in dogs.},
copyright = {2022 The Author(s)},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Animal behaviour,Learning and memory,Social behaviour},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Junttila et al/junttila_et_al_2022_breed_differences_in_social_cognition,_inhibitory_control,_and_spatial.pdf}
}
@article{Kelly.etal.2019,
title = {Motoric self-regulation by sled dogs and pet dogs and the acute effect of carbohydrate source in sled dogs},
author = {Kelly, Debbie M. and Adolphe, Jennifer L. and Vernouillet, Aliz{\'e}e and McCausland, J. Andrew and Rankovic, Alexandra and Verbrugghe, Adronie},
year = {2019},
month = jun,
journal = {Animal Cognition},
volume = {22},
number = {6},
pages = {931--946},
issn = {1435-9456},
doi = {10.1007/s10071-019-01285-y},
urldate = {2019-07-11},
abstract = {Inhibitory control is a term used to envelop a collection of processes that allow an organism to refrain from engaging in an inappropriate prepotent or responsive behavior. Studies have examined the propensity of inhibitory control by nonhuman animals, from the cognitively complex processes involved in self-control to potentially less cognitively taxing processes such as motoric self-regulation. Focusing on canines, research has suggested that the domestication process as well as experiences during ontogeny contribute to inhibitory control. Diet may also play an important role in an individual's ability to self-regulate. This study examined this possibility by investigating motoric self-regulation in sled dogs, using three well-established tasks (i.e., A-not-B Bucket, Cylinder, and A-not-B Barrier tasks), performed after consumption of one of three dietary treatments with different glycemic index values. We also compared the performance of sled dogs during these tasks with results previously obtained from pet dogs. Overall, the results show many similarities in the performance of sled dogs and pet dogs on the motoric self-regulation tasks, with the notable exception that sled dogs may have a stronger spatial perseveration during the A-not-B Bucket task. Previous research findings reporting a lack of correlation among these tasks are also supported. Finally, during the early postprandial phase (period after consumption), dietary treatments with different glycemic index values did not influence self-regulatory performance for sled dogs.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Carbohydrate,Glycemic index,Inhibitory control,Motoric self-regulation,Sled dogs},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Kelly et al/kelly_et_al_2019_motoric_self-regulation_by_sled_dogs_and_pet_dogs_and_the_acute_effect_of.pdf}
}
@article{Knudson.Lindsey.2014,
title = {Type {{I}} and type {{II}} errors in correlations of various sample sizes},
author = {Knudson, Duane V. and Lindsey, Crawford},
year = {2014},
month = jan,
journal = {Comprehensive Psychology},
volume = {3},
pages = {03.CP.3.1},
publisher = {{SAGE Publications Inc}},
issn = {2165-2228},
doi = {10.2466/03.CP.3.1},
urldate = {2022-12-21},
abstract = {This study documented the effect of sample sizes commonly seen in exercise science research on type I and type II errors in statistical tests of numerous correlations. Data on tennis string testing were used to examine zero-order and partial correlations between six variables for the population (N = 198) and three randomly drawn sub-samples of 99, 50, and 25. Sample size and statistical analysis procedure affected the rates of statistical errors. Reducing sample size increased type II errors 7\% to 21\% using correlation analysis. Partial correlation analysis of smaller samples increased type II errors 29\% to 85\%. Correlation studies of small sample sizes are likely vulnerable to type I or type II statistical errors and should be interpreted with caution.},
langid = {english},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Knudson et al/knudson_et_al_2014_type_i_and_type_ii_errors_in_correlations_of_various_sample_sizes.pdf}
}
@article{Kruger.etal.2023,
title = {What do evolutionary researchers believe about human psychology and behavior?},
author = {Kruger, Daniel J. and Fisher, Maryanne L. and Salmon, Catherine},
year = {2023},
month = jan,
journal = {Evolution and Human Behavior},
volume = {44},
number = {1},
pages = {11--18},
issn = {1090-5138},
doi = {10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.11.002},
urldate = {2022-12-23},
abstract = {We investigated the prevalence of beliefs in several key and contested aspects of human psychology and behavior in a broad sample of evolutionary-informed scholars (N~=~581). Nearly all participants believed that developmental environments substantially shape human adult psychology and behavior, that there are differences in human psychology and behavior based on sex differences from sexual selection, and that there are individual differences in human psychology and behavior resulting from different genotypes. About three-quarters of participants believed that there are population differences from dissimilar ancestral ecologies/environments and within-person differences across the menstrual cycle. Three-fifths believed that the human mind consists of domain-specific, context-sensitive modules. About half of participants believed that behavioral and cognitive aspects of human life history vary along a unified fast-slow continuum. Two-fifths of participants believed that group-level selection has substantially contributed to human evolution. Results indicate that there are both shared core beliefs as well as phenomena that are accepted by varying proportions of scholars. Such patterns represent the views of contemporary scholars and the current state of the field. The degree of acceptance for some phenomena may change over time as evolutionary science advances through the accumulation of empirical evidence.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Beliefs,Evolutionary psychology,Group selection,Life history,Menstrual cycle},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Kruger et al/kruger_et_al_2023_what_do_evolutionary_researchers_believe_about_human_psychology_and_behavior.pdf}
}
@article{Kubinyi.etal.2012,
title = {Polymorphism in the tyrosine hydroxylase ({{TH}}) gene is associated with activity-impulsivity in {{German Shepherd Dogs}}},
author = {Kubinyi, Enik{\H o} and Vas, Judit and Hejjas, Krisztina and Ronai, Zsolt and Br{\'u}der, Ildik{\'o} and Turcs{\'a}n, Borb{\'a}la and {Sasvari-Szekely}, Maria and Mikl{\'o}si, {\'A}d{\'a}m},
year = {2012},
month = jan,
journal = {PLOS ONE},
volume = {7},
number = {1},
pages = {e30271},
publisher = {{Public Library of Science}},
issn = {1932-6203},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0030271},
urldate = {2022-03-08},
abstract = {We investigated the association between repeat polymorphism in intron 4 of the tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) gene and two personality traits, activity-impulsivity and inattention, in German Shepherd Dogs. The behaviour of 104 dogs was characterized by two instruments: (1) the previously validated Dog-Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale (Dog-ADHD RS) filled in by the dog owners and (2) the newly developed Activity-impulsivity Behavioural Scale (AIBS) containing four subtests, scored by the experimenters. Internal consistency, inter-observer reliability, test-retest reliability and convergent validity were demonstrated for AIBS. Dogs possessing at least one short allele were proved to be more active-impulsive by both instruments, compared to dogs carrying two copies of the long allele (activity-impulsivity scale of Dog-ADHD RS: p = 0.007; AIBS: p = 0.023). The results have some potential to support human studies; however, further research should reveal the molecular function of the TH gene variants, and look for the effect in more breeds.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Alleles,Animal behavior,Animal sexual behavior,Dogs,Introns,Pets and companion animals,Tyrosine,Variant genotypes},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Kubinyi et al/kubinyi_et_al_2012_polymorphism_in_the_tyrosine_hydroxylase_(th)_gene_is_associated_with.pdf}
}
@article{Lazarowski.etal.2020a,
title = {The development of problem-solving abilities in a population of candidate detection dogs ({{{\emph{Canis}}}}{\emph{ familiaris}})},
author = {Lazarowski, Lucia and Krichbaum, Sarah and Waggoner, L. Paul and Katz, Jeffrey S.},
year = {2020},
month = apr,
journal = {Animal Cognition},
volume = {23},
pages = {755--768},
issn = {1435-9456},
doi = {10.1007/s10071-020-01387-y},
urldate = {2020-04-24},
abstract = {Both ontogenetic and phylogenetic factors have shaped dogs' cognitive capabilities, resulting in a heightened social sensitivity at the apparent cost of non-social problem-solving abilities. Research has suggested that training history and life experience can influence problem-solving abilities in dogs. However, the ontogenetic development of problem-solving abilities in dogs has not been explored. We tested a population of candidate detection dogs of various ages across the first year of development on four well-established problem-solving tasks targeting different cognitive domains (i.e., cylinder, A-not-B barrier, delayed search, and spatial transposition tasks). We examined developmental effects by comparing cognitive task performance across three age groups. Age-related improvements for all four cognitive measures indicate developmental increases in processes related to inhibitory control, attention, and spatial cognition between 3 and 12~months of age. Additionally, we found some relationships between cognitive measures and detection dog performance measures, though effects were not as robust. We discuss the results in the context of canine cognitive development and corresponding effects of phylogeny and ontogeny, as well as potential applications to working dog training and selection.},
langid = {english}
}
@article{Logan.etal.2022,
title = {Are the more flexible great-tailed grackles also better at inhibition?},
author = {Logan, C J and McCune, K B and MacPherson, M and {Johnson-Ulrich}, Z and Rowney, C and Seitz, B and Blaisdell, A P and Deffner, D and Wascher, C A F},
year = {2022},
journal = {Animal Behavior and Cognition},
volume = {9},
number = {1},
pages = {14--36},
doi = {10.26451/abc.09.01.03.2022},
abstract = {Behavioral flexibility should, theoretically, be positively related to behavioral inhibition because one should need to inhibit a previously learned behavior to change their behavior when the task changes (flexibility). However, several investigations show no or mixed support of this hypothesis, which challenges the assumption that inhibition is involved in making flexible decisions. We tested the hypothesis that flexibility (reversal learning and solution switching on a multi-access box by Logan et al., 2022) is associated with inhibition (go/no go on a touchscreen and detour) by measuring all variables in the same individuals. Because touchscreen experiments had never been conducted in this species, we validated that they are functional for wild-caught grackles who learned to use it and completed go/no go on it. Performance on go/no go and detour tasks did not correlate, indicating they did not measure the same trait. Individuals who were faster to reverse took more time to attempt a new option on the multi-access box and were either faster or slower at go/no go depending on whether one individual, Taquito (accidentally tested beyond 200 trial cap), was included in the GLM. While the relationship between trials to reverse and trials to pass go/no go was strongly influenced by Taquito, the more comprehensive Bayesian flexibility model supported the positive relationship irrespective of whether Taquito was included. Performance on detour did not correlate with either flexibility measure, suggesting that they may measure separate traits. We conclude that flexibility is associated with certain types of inhibition, but not others, in great-tailed grackles.}
}
@article{MacKillop.etal.2016,
title = {The latent structure of impulsivity: impulsive choice, impulsive action, and impulsive personality traits},
shorttitle = {The latent structure of impulsivity},
author = {MacKillop, James and Weafer, Jessica and C. Gray, Joshua and Oshri, Assaf and Palmer, Abraham and {de Wit}, Harriet},
year = {2016},
month = sep,
journal = {Psychopharmacology},
volume = {233},
number = {18},
pages = {3361--3370},
issn = {1432-2072},
doi = {10.1007/s00213-016-4372-0},
urldate = {2022-12-19},
abstract = {Impulsivity has been strongly linked to addictive behaviors, but can be operationalized in a number of ways that vary considerably in overlap, suggesting multidimensionality.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Addiction,Behavioral inhibition,Confirmatory factor analysis,Delay discounting,Impulsive personality,Impulsivity},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/MacKillop et al/mackillop_et_al_2016_the_latent_structure_of_impulsivity.pdf}
}
@article{Maejima.etal.2007,
title = {Traits and genotypes may predict the successful training of drug detection dogs},
author = {Maejima, Masami and {Inoue-Murayama}, Miho and Tonosaki, Keiichi and Matsuura, Naoto and Kato, Shota and Saito, Yasuhiro and Weiss, Alexander and Murayama, Yuichi and Ito, Shin'ichi},
year = {2007},
month = nov,
journal = {Applied Animal Behaviour Science},
volume = {107},
number = {3},
pages = {287--298},
issn = {0168-1591},
doi = {10.1016/j.applanim.2006.10.005},
urldate = {2022-12-10},
abstract = {In Japan, approximately 30\% of dogs that enter training programs to become drug detection dogs successfully complete training. To clarify factors related to the aptitude of drug detection dogs and develop an assessment tool, we evaluated genotypes and behavioural traits of 197 candidate dogs. The behavioural traits were evaluated within 2 weeks from the start of training and included general activity, obedience training, concentration, affection demand, aggression toward dogs, anxiety, and interest in target. Principal components analysis of these ratings yielded two components: Desire for Work and Distractibility. Desire for Work was significantly related to successful completion of training (P{$<$}0.001). Since 93.3\% of dogs that passed training and 53.3\% of the dogs that failed training had Desire for Work scores of 45 or higher, we will be able to reject about half of inappropriate dogs before 3 months of training by adopting this cut-off point. We also surveyed eight polymorphic regions of four genes that have been related to human personality dimensions. Genotypes were not related to whether dogs passed, but there was a weak relationship between Distractibility and a 5HTT haplotype (P{$<$}0.05).},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Behavioural traits,Drug detection dog,Genotype,Training},
file = {/home/jstevens/Zotero/storage/FNEQ8VKF/Maejima et al. - 2007 - Traits and genotypes may predict the successful tr.pdf;/home/jstevens/Zotero/storage/VDK52XHH/S0168159106003418.html}
}
@article{Maier.etal.2023,
title = {Robust {{Bayesian}} meta-analysis: {{Addressing}} publication bias with model-averaging},
shorttitle = {Robust {{Bayesian}} meta-analysis},
author = {Maier, Maximilian and Barto{\v s}, Franti{\v s}ek and Wagenmakers, Eric-Jan},
year = {2023},
journal = {Psychological Methods},
volume = {28},
number = {1},
pages = {107--122},
publisher = {{American Psychological Association}},
issn = {1082-989X},
doi = {10.1037/met0000405},
urldate = {2022-06-07},
abstract = {Meta-analysis is an important quantitative tool for cumulative science, but its application is frustrated by publication bias. In order to test and adjust for publication bias, we extend model-averaged Bayesian meta-analysis with selection models. The resulting robust Bayesian meta-analysis (RoBMA) methodology does not require all-or-none decisions about the presence of publication bias, can quantify evidence in favor of the absence of publication bias, and performs well under high heterogeneity. By model-averaging over a set of 12 models, RoBMA is relatively robust to model misspecification and simulations show that it outperforms existing methods. We demonstrate that RoBMA finds evidence for the absence of publication bias in Registered Replication Reports and reliably avoids false positives. We provide an implementation in R so that researchers can easily use the new methodology in practice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved)},
keywords = {Adjustment,Bayesian Analysis,Errors,evidence,heterogeneity,Meta Analysis,Models,Sciences,selection models,Simulation,Statistical Probability},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Maier et al/maier_et_al_2022_robust_bayesian_meta-analysis.pdf}
}
@article{Malle.Neubauer.1991,
title = {Impulsivity, reflection, and questionnaire response latencies: {{No}} evidence for a broad impulsivity trait},
shorttitle = {Impulsivity, reflection, and questionnaire response latencies},
author = {Malle, Bertram F. and Neubauer, Aljoscha C.},
year = {1991},
month = jan,
journal = {Personality and Individual Differences},
volume = {12},
number = {8},
pages = {865--871},
issn = {0191-8869},
doi = {10.1016/0191-8869(91)90153-3},
urldate = {2022-12-19},
abstract = {Impulsivity is generally held to be an important dimension of personality. It has been conceptualized in many different ways: as a cognitive style termed impulsivity/reflection, as a subtrait of extraversion or psychoticism, and as a trait of its own. The present study focused on the relation between some of these different impulsivity aspects and response latencies to questionnaires. The Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT), the Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI), a self-report impulsivity questionnaire, and the Self-Consciousness Scale (SCS) were administered to 65 Austrian undergraduate students. By means of a computerized presentation, questionnaire response latencies were recorded. Virtually no relationship was found between MFFT, self-report impulsivity, and response latencies. In addition to previous studies, the present results, therefore, cast doubt on a broad impulsivity concept. Secondary findings of this study are pertinent to the relation between impulsivity and anxiety, to the orthogonality of impulsivity and self-consciousness, and to sex differences in impulsivity.},
langid = {english}
}
@misc{ManyDogsProject.etal.2022,
title = {{{ManyDogs Project}}: {{A}} big team science approach to investigating canine behavior and cognition},
shorttitle = {{{ManyDogs}} project},
author = {{ManyDogs Project} and Alberghina, Daniela and Bray, Emily and Buchsbaum, Daphna and Byosiere, Sarah-Elizabeth and Espinosa, Julia and Gnanadesikan, Gitanjali and Guran, C.-N. Alexandrina and Hare, Elizabeth and Horschler, Daniel and Huber, Ludwig and Kuhlmeier, Valerie A. and MacLean, Evan and Pelgrim, Madeline H. and Perez, Bryan and {Ravid-Schurr}, Dana and Rothkoff, Liza and Sexton, Courtney and Silver, Zachary and Stevens, Jeffrey R.},
year = {2022},
month = sep,
publisher = {{PsyArXiv}},
doi = {10.31234/osf.io/j82uc},
urldate = {2022-09-02},
abstract = {Although dogs have a special place in human history as the first domesticated species and play important roles in many cultures around the world, their role in scientific studies has been relatively recent. With a few notable exceptions (e.g., Darwin, Pavlov, Scott \& Fuller), domestic dogs were not commonly the subject of rigorous scientific investigation of behavior until the late 1990s. While the number of canine science studies has increased dramatically over the last 20 years, most research groups are limited in the inferences they can draw due to the relatively small sample sizes used along with the exceptional diversity observed in dogs (e.g., breed, geographic location, experience). To this end, we introduce the ManyDogs Project, an international consortium of researchers interested in taking a big team science approach to understanding canine behavioral science. We begin by discussing why studying dogs provides valuable insights into behavior and cognition, evolutionary processes, human health, and applications for animal welfare. We then highlight other big team science projects that have previously been conducted in canine science and emphasize the benefits of our approach. Finally, we introduce the ManyDogs Project and our mission: 1) replicating important findings, 2) investigating moderators that need a large sample size such as breed differences, 3) reaching methodological consensus, 4) investigating cross-cultural differences, and finally 5) setting a standard for replication studies in general. In doing so, we hope to address previous limitations in individual lab studies and previous big team science frameworks to deepen our understanding of canine behavior and cognition.},
langid = {american},
keywords = {animal behavior,Animal Learning and Behavior,big team science,canine cognition,canine science,collaboration,comparative cognition,Dogs,domestic dogs,ManyDogs,Open Science,Replication,reproducibility,Social and Behavioral Sciences},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/ManyDogsProject et al/manydogsproject_et_al_2022_manydogs_project.pdf}
}
@article{Marshall-Pescini.etal.2015,
title = {The effect of domestication on inhibitory control: wolves and dogs compared},
shorttitle = {The {{Effect}} of {{Domestication}} on {{Inhibitory Control}}},
author = {{Marshall-Pescini}, Sarah and Vir{\'a}nyi, Zs{\'o}fia and Range, Friederike},
year = {2015},
month = feb,
journal = {PLOS ONE},
volume = {10},
number = {2},
pages = {e0118469},
issn = {1932-6203},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0118469},
urldate = {2017-06-06},
abstract = {Inhibitory control i.e. blocking an impulsive or prepotent response in favour of a more appropriate alternative, has been suggested to play an important role in cooperative behaviour. Interestingly, while dogs and wolves show a similar social organization, they differ in their intraspecific cooperation tendencies in that wolves rely more heavily on group coordination in regard to hunting and pup-rearing compared to dogs. Hence, based on the `canine cooperation' hypothesis wolves should show better inhibitory control than dogs. On the other hand, through the domestication process, dogs may have been selected for cooperative tendencies towards humans and/or a less reactive temperament, which may in turn have affected their inhibitory control abilities. Hence, based on the latter hypothesis, we would expect dogs to show a higher performance in tasks requiring inhibitory control. To test the predictive value of these alternative hypotheses, in the current study two tasks; the `cylinder task' and the `detour task', which are designed to assess inhibitory control, were used to evaluate the performance of identically raised pack dogs and wolves. Results from the cylinder task showed a significantly poorer performance in wolves than identically-raised pack dogs (and showed that pack-dogs performed similarly to pet dogs with different training experiences), however contrary results emerged in the detour task, with wolves showing a shorter latency to success and less perseverative behaviour at the fence. Results are discussed in relation to previous studies using these paradigms and in terms of the validity of these two methods in assessing inhibitory control.},
keywords = {a,b,c,D,e,f,g,h,i,l,m,n,o,P,r,s,t,u,v,W,y}
}
@article{Mongillo.etal.2019,
title = {Development of a spatial discount task to measure impulsive choices in dogs},
author = {Mongillo, Paolo and Scandurra, Anna and Eatherington, Carla Jade and D'Aniello, Biagio and Marinelli, Lieta},
year = {2019},
month = jul,
journal = {Animals},
volume = {9},
number = {7},
pages = {469},
doi = {10.3390/ani9070469},
urldate = {2019-07-29},
abstract = {Impulsive choices reflect an individual\’s tendency to prefer a smaller immediate reward over a larger delayed one. Here, we have developed a behavioural test which can be easily applied to assess impulsive choices in dogs. Dogs were trained to associate one of two equidistant locations with a larger food amount when a smaller amount was presented in the other location, then the smaller amount was placed systematically closer to the dog. Choices of the smaller amount, as a function of distance, were considered a measure of the dog\’s tendency to make impulsive choices. All dogs (N = 48) passed the learning phase and completed the entire assessment in under 1 h. Choice of the smaller food amount increased as this was placed closer to the dog. Choices were independent from food motivation, past training, and speed of learning the training phase; supporting the specificity of the procedure. Females showed a higher probability of making impulsive choices, in agreement with analogue sex differences found in human and rodent studies, and supporting the external validity of our assessment. Overall, the findings support the practical applicability and represent a first indication of the validity of this method, making it suitable for investigations into impulsivity in dogs.},
copyright = {http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/},
langid = {english},
keywords = {behavioral test,dog,impulsivity,learning,sex differences,validation},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Mongillo et al/mongillo_et_al_2019_development_of_a_spatial_discount_task_to_measure_impulsive_choices_in_dogs2.pdf}
}
@article{Moreau.Gamble.2022,
title = {Conducting a meta-analysis in the age of open science: {{Tools}}, tips, and practical recommendations.},
shorttitle = {Conducting a meta-analysis in the age of open science},
author = {Moreau, David and Gamble, Beau},
year = {2022},
journal = {Psychological Methods},
volume = {27},
number = {3},
pages = {426--432},
issn = {1939-1463, 1082-989X},
doi = {10.1037/met0000351},
urldate = {2022-05-11},
abstract = {Psychology researchers are rapidly adopting open science practices, yet clear guidelines on how to apply these practices to meta-analysis remain lacking. In this tutorial, we describe why open science is important in the context of meta-analysis in psychology, and suggest how to adopt the 3 main components of open science: preregistration, open materials, and open data. We first describe how to make the preregistration as thorough as possible\textemdash and how to handle deviations from the plan. We then focus on creating easy-to-read materials (e.g., search syntax, R scripts) to facilitate reproducibility and bolster the impact of a meta-analysis. Finally, we suggest how to organize data (e.g., literature search results, data extracted from studies) that are easy to share, interpret, and update as new studies emerge. For each step of the meta-analysis, we provide example templates, accompanied by brief video tutorials, and show how to integrate these practices into the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/q8stz/).},
langid = {english}
}
@article{Muller.etal.2016,
title = {Inhibitory control, but not prolonged object-related experience appears to affect physical problem-solving performance of pet dogs},
author = {M{\"u}ller, Corsin A. and Riemer, Stefanie and Vir{\'a}nyi, Zs{\'o}fia and Huber, Ludwig and Range, Friederike},
year = {2016},
month = feb,
journal = {PLOS ONE},
volume = {11},
number = {2},
pages = {e0147753},
issn = {1932-6203},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0147753},
urldate = {2018-02-23},
abstract = {Human infants develop an understanding of their physical environment through playful interactions with objects. Similar processes may influence also the performance of non-human animals in physical problem-solving tasks, but to date there is little empirical data to evaluate this hypothesis. In addition or alternatively to prior experiences, inhibitory control has been suggested as a factor underlying the considerable individual differences in performance reported for many species. Here we report a study in which we manipulated the extent of object-related experience for a cohort of dogs (Canis familiaris) of the breed Border Collie over a period of 18 months, and assessed their level of inhibitory control, prior to testing them in a series of four physical problem-solving tasks. We found no evidence that differences in object-related experience explain variability in performance in these tasks. It thus appears that dogs do not transfer knowledge about physical rules from one physical problem-solving task to another, but rather approach each task as a novel problem. Our results, however, suggest that individual performance in these tasks is influenced in a complex way by the subject's level of inhibitory control. Depending on the task, inhibitory control had a positive or a negative effect on performance and different aspects of inhibitory control turned out to be the best predictors of individual performance in the different tasks. Therefore, studying the interplay between inhibitory control and problem-solving performance will make an important contribution to our understanding of individual and species differences in physical problem-solving performance.},
langid = {english},
keywords = {Animal cognition,Animal performance,Binomials,Dogs,Domestic animals,Learning,Pets and companion animals,Primates}
}
@article{Nudelman.Otto.2020,
title = {The development of a new generic risk-of-bias measure for systematic reviews of surveys},
author = {Nudelman, Gabriel and Otto, Kathleen},
year = {2020},
month = dec,
journal = {Methodology},
volume = {16},
number = {4},
pages = {278--298},
issn = {1614-2241},
doi = {10.5964/meth.4329},
urldate = {2022-12-20},
abstract = {It is important to evaluate risk of bias of the primary studies included in systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Since tools pertinent to surveys are scarce, the goal of the current research was to develop a measure to address this need. In Study 1, an initial list of 10 relevant topics was compiled from previous measures. In Study 2, the list was refined into an eight-item risk-of-bias measure via discussion and a pilot study. In Study 3, experienced researchers used the measure to asses 70 studies, demonstrating high interrater agreement (weighted Kappa = .82). Inexperienced raters also utilized the measure to code 26 different studies included in a prior meta-analysis, which resulted in adequate interrater agreement (weighted Kappa = .64) and excellent convergent validity (r = .66). Thus, the new measure, designed to be accessible and flexible, can increase standardization of risk-of-bias evaluations and contribute to the interpretation of systematic reviews and meta-analytic findings.},
copyright = {Copyright (c) 2020 Gabriel Nudelman, Kathleen Otto},
langid = {english},
keywords = {correlational studies,meta-analysis,risk of bias,survey quality,systematic review},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Nudelman et al/nudelman_et_al_2020_the_development_of_a_new_generic_risk-of-bias_measure_for_systematic_reviews_of.pdf}
}
@article{Olsen.2018,
title = {A case for methodological overhaul and increased study of executive function in the domestic dog ({{{\emph{Canis}}}}{\emph{ lupus familiaris}})},
author = {Olsen, Mariana R.},
year = {2018},
month = mar,
journal = {Animal Cognition},
volume = {21},
number = {2},
pages = {175--195},
issn = {1435-9448, 1435-9456},
doi = {10.1007/s10071-018-1162-6},
urldate = {2018-02-23},
abstract = {Executive function (EF) allows for self-regulation of behavior including maintaining focus in the face of distraction, inhibiting behavior that is suboptimal or inappropriate in a given context, and updating the contents of working memory. While EF has been studied extensively in humans, it has only recently become a topic of research in the domestic dog. In this paper, I argue for increased study of dog EF by explaining how it might influence the owner\textendash dog bond, human safety, and dog welfare, as well as reviewing the current literature dedicated to EF in dogs. In ``EF and its Application to ``Man's Best Friend'' section, I briefly describe EF and how it is relevant to dog behavior. In ``Previous investigations into EF in dogs'' section, I provide a review of the literature pertaining to EF in dogs, specifically tasks used to assess abilities like inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and working memory capacity. In ``Insights and limitations of previous studies'' section, I consider limitations of existing studies that must be addressed in future research. Finally, in ``Future directions'' section, I propose future directions for meaningful research on EF in dogs.},
langid = {english},
file = {/home/jstevens/zotero-library/Olsen/olsen_2018_a_case_for_methodological_overhaul_and_increased_study_of_executive_function_in2.pdf}
}
@phdthesis{Olsen.2019,
title = {Does {{Increased Task Difficulty Reveal Individual Differences}} in {{Executive Function}} in the {{Domestic Dog}}?},
author = {Olsen, Mariana Rachel},
year = {2019},
address = {{United States -- Montana}},
urldate = {2022-08-09},
abstract = {Pet dogs are carnivores that inhabit a largely human-dominated context, in which certain normal canid behaviors (e.g., resource-guarding, barking, mounting) are considered undesirable and even dangerous. Safety and welfare implications of human-dog interaction have recently led researchers to take an interest in canine executive function. Two tasks have become particularly popular in this area of study: the cylinder task and the A-not-B task. Because canine cognition tasks are not typically subjected to the same scrutiny as those used in human research, it is unclear whether these tasks indeed measure what researchers expect them to. Even though they ostensibly measure canine inhibitory control, task performance seldom correlates between the two, and researchers have suggested that they might be too easy to reflect effortful processes. Further complicating the matter are lack of reliability estimates and frequent use of under-powered samples. In this study, I evaluated the reliability and construct validity of the cylinder task and A-not-B task. Across two experiments, I tested modified forms of the cylinder task to make it more difficult and thus more reflective of individual differences in executive function. In Experiment 1, subjects completed the cylinder task under normal conditions and following self-control exertion. In Experiment 2, subjects performed the cylinder task either with or without practice retrieving a treat from an opaque apparatus. Subjects in both experiments performed the A-not-B task with removal of ostensive human cuing. Performance on behavioral tasks was compared to owner-reported measures of impulsivity, inattention, behavioral regulation, responsiveness, and aggression. In Experiment 1, performance was negatively affected by self-control exertion, but only to the degree that dogs exhibited self-control. This suggests that the cylinder task reflects an effortful, limited-capacity process. In Experiment 2, subjects performed worse when practice was omitted, suggesting that cylinder task performance partially reflects the ability to transfer the strategy learned during practice to the test trials. Across both studies, performance during the cylinder task and A-not-B task was uncorrelated. Further, the cylinder task showed high reliability whereas the A-not-B did not. Implications of these results and suggestions for future directions are discussed.},
copyright = {Database copyright ProQuest LLC; ProQuest does not claim copyright in the individual underlying works.},
isbn = {9780438784628},
langid = {english},
school = {Montana State University},
keywords = {Canine,Cognition,Inhibition,Psychology}
}
@article{Parrish.etal.2018,
title = {Investigating the depletion effect: {{Self-control}} does not waiver in capuchin monkeys},
author = {Parrish, {\relax AE} and James, {\relax BT} and Rossettie, {\relax MS} and Smith, T and {Otalora-Garcia}, A and Beran, {\relax MJ}},
year = {2018},
journal = {Animal Behavior and Cognition},
volume = {5},
number = {4},
pages = {118--138},
doi = {10.26451/abc.05.01.09.2018},
abstract = {The ego-depletion hypothesis states that self-control diminishes over time and with exertion. However, there is mixed evidence among human adult and comparative studies as to whether such depletion occurs. It is an important issue, given that evidence for or against this hypothesis could have implications for remediation efforts with individuals who show high impulsivity and low self-control. In a study of potential depletion effects on self-control, capuchin monkeys were presented with two consecutive self-control tasks back-to-back within sessions. Monkeys first completed the accumulation task, in which they were presented with food items one-by-one until the subject retrieved and ate the accumulating items, at which point no more food would be delivered. This required continual inhibition of food retrieval in the face of an increasingly desirable reward. Then, monkeys completed a food exchange task with exchange combinations that either decreased or increased in food quality. Self-control was required in foregoing eating an immediately available food for a potentially better reward later in the trial. Individual differences in accumulation performance were observed, but no depletion effects were seen in the monkeys' exchange performance. Next, monkeys were presented with task order counterbalanced across individuals. No order effects were observed in the monkeys' performance on either self-control task. Monkeys' exchange performance was not significantly correlated with accumulation performance in either experiment. These results indicate no depletion effects and that these tasks may not be related in terms of underlying mechanisms that support self-control performance, even though at face value both require inhibition of eating available food.}
}